Schizophrenia Research Forum - A Catalyst for Creative Thinking
Home Profile Membership/Get Newsletter Log In Contact Us
 For Patients & Families
What's New
Recent Updates
SRF Papers
Current Papers
Search All Papers
Search Comments
News
Research News
Conference News
Plain English
Forums
Current Hypotheses
Idea Lab
Online Discussions
Virtual Conferences
Interviews
Resources
What We Know
SchizophreniaGene
Animal Models
Drugs in Trials
Research Tools
Grants
Jobs
Conferences
Journals
Community Calendar
General Information
Community
Member Directory
Researcher Profiles
Institutes and Labs
About the Site
Mission
History
SRF Team
Advisory Board
Support Us
How to Cite
Fan (E)Mail
The Schizophrenia Research Forum web site is sponsored by the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation and was created with funding from the U.S. National Institute of Mental Health.
Research News
back to News Search Plain English
A Model Is a Model Is a Model of Mental Illness?

October 31, 2013. Apologies to Gertrude Stein and her rose, but the paraphrase of the title is apt when discussing genetically altered mice asserted to be models of mental illness. Two new, high-profile papers bring this issue to the forefront.

In a study appearing October 16 in Neuron, researchers in Susumu Tonegawa's laboratory at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology in Cambridge report that ensembles of hippocampal neurons in a transgenic mouse lacking calcineurin in its forebrain do not appear to "replay" recently learned maze information as wild-type mice do. They suggest that this might underlie some of the memory and behavioral abnormalities of the mouse, but more importantly, that it represents a mouse model of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, as they had previously published evidence for calcineurin as a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia.

In another new study, published October 23 in Nature, Huda Zoghbi and colleagues at Baylor College of Medicine in Houston, Texas, suggest that they have found an animal model of bipolar mania in "hyperkinetic" mice who carry an extra copy of the gene for Shank3, causing them to overexpress the protein. To this evidence, they add two patients with duplications of the gene for Shank3 who have complex neuropsychiatric disorders that include hyperactivity or bipolar disorder.

The two studies elicit quite different reactions from other researchers. In a Neuron Preview of the calcineurin transgenic mouse, Patricio O'Donnell of Pfizer, Inc., in Cambridge, Massachusetts, writes that "the loss of awake replay in calcineurin knockout mice provides a glimpse into what could be fundamental mechanisms perhaps relevant to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and related disorders."

Francis McMahon of the National Institute of Mental Health in Bethesda, Maryland, takes a different view of the "bipolar mania" model in a comment for Schizophrenia Research Forum. He writes that despite the informative characterization of Shank3 overexpressing mice, "the paper suffers from a too strong reliance on partial behavioral resemblances with mania in humans."

The ripple replay
The Tonegawa lab produced the original research suggesting a link between calcineurin and schizophrenia risk, and collaborator David J. Foster of Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, Maryland, supplied the technique that allowed the researchers to look for neural processing abnormalities in the transgenic mice. Among other researchers over the past decade, Foster has helped to describe neuronal ensembles in the hippocampus that encode spatial maps—so-called "place cells." The key clues have been electrophysiologic patterns, both on EEG and in single neuron recordings, that appear to represent the brain replaying spatial information, presumably to store it in memory.

However, in his editorial on the paper, O'Donnell writes that such replay "relates to more than memory but to pondering of different scenarios evaluated in the learning process." As such, he suggests it could be a model for how cognition works more generally in the brain and thus be a useful probe for cognitive deficits such as those in schizophrenia.

In their study, Foster, co-first author Junghyup Suh of MIT, and their colleagues found that the calcineurin forebrain knockout mice have apparently normal levels of neural activity in the hippocampus as they navigate a maze, yet overactive hippocampi during awake resting periods. This was true both for a hippocampus-wide EEG pattern, termed "sharp-wave ripple," linked to the replay of spatial memory and for the firing of individual neurons. Moreover, Suh and colleagues found that the hippocampal neurons in the calcineurin knockout mice lacked the ordered repetition of place cell firing patterns seen in wild-type mice which apparently replay spatial information while resting.

"Thus, we present a novel form of disruption of neural information processing in an animal model of schizophrenia," write the authors. A critical open question, which the authors do not address, is whether variation in the gene for calcineurin will be confirmed as a risk factor for the disease in the era of genomewide association as the generally accepted standard.

Hyperkinetic
Point mutations and deletions in the three Shank genes have also been linked to schizophrenia, as well as autism and intellectual disability (see related SRF related news story). There are also a few duplications of the genomic region 22q13, containing Shank3, reported in people with mental illness. First author Kihoon Han and colleagues decided to see what effect Shank3 overexpression would have. Their transgenic mice produce ~50 percent more Shank3 than wild-type animals do and are overactive. This would seem to indicate that they might be modeling some aspect of ADHD, but when they gave the mice amphetamine, which can have a calming effect in ADHD, they instead produced even more activity. This suggested to the researchers that they might instead be modeling mania, which is increased by amphetamine.

Various animal tests and behaviors proposed to model mania—activity during tail suspension, acoustic startle abnormalities, abnormal circadian rhythms, increased appetite—pointed in the predicted direction, leading the researchers to propose that they were modeling the mania of human bipolar disorder. One drug effective in some cases of mania, valproate, was effective in normalizing some of these measures, but lithium was not.

The transgenic mice had a number of biological abnormalities, including alterations in the ratio of excitatory to inhibitory signaling (E/I balance) in cultured hippocampal neurons toward more excitation (which has been reported to underlie seizures). When Han and colleagues explored the large number of proteins interacting with Shank3, they zeroed in on proteins regulating the actin cytoskeleton as their main suspects for the E/I alterations. They write that their data suggest that Shank3 acts as a scaffold that allows other proteins to form F-actin.

More on modeling
Han and colleagues found supporting evidence for the idea that the mice model mania in the form of two human patients with multiple neurobehavioral conditions but very circumscribed 22q13 duplications, including Shank3, encompassing two and three genes, respectively. One was diagnosed with ADHD and other neurobehavioral deficits, while the other had a bipolar diagnosis, and both had seizure disorders. They assert, "These findings indicate that Shank3 overexpression causes a hyperkinetic neuropsychiatric disorder."

However, the question of whether this represents a model of mania runs up against data that do not conform: McMahon points out that "Shank3 overexpressing mice also displayed decreased social interaction, decreased ultrasonic vocalization, and lack of response to lithium, which are strongly inconsistent with mania. Hedonic behavior and aggression were not reported."

In the quest to explain the molecular basis of mental illness, McMahon is placing his bets on in-vitro models based on induced pluripotent stem cells derived from people with psychiatric disorders. This, he maintains, may allow us "to make a clean break from anthropomorphic dependence on behavioral 'resemblances' and move toward the molecular components which are truly conserved across organisms."

Vis-à-vis the study by Han and colleagues, on the other hand, O'Donnell argues that it represents a credible model for schizophrenia research in part because it is not purported to represent the primary diagnostic phenotype of hallucinations and delusions. Instead, the researchers are examining neural activity underlying a specific memory function, one that may help explain some aspects of disordered thought in psychotic disorders. "[M]odels should be viewed … as reagents to probe specific questions about etiological and pathophysiological pathways," he writes.

Traffic is increasing on two avenues for research into psychiatric disorders—animal and in-vitro models are increasing in numbers and ease of use, and will soon intersect with a virtual rush hour of many, perhaps hundreds of risk genes of very small effect. It will be interesting to see whether the two merge seamlessly to produce new insights and treatments, or produce a pile-up of too much uninterpretable data.—Hakon Heimer.

References:
Suh J, Foster DJ, Davoudi H, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. Impaired hippocampal ripple-associated replay in a mouse model of schizophrenia. Neuron. 2013 Oct 16; 80(2):484-93. Abstract

O'Donnell P. Of Mice and Men: What Physiological Correlates of Cognitive Deficits in a Mouse Model of Schizophrenia Tell Us about Psychiatric Disease. Neuron. 2013 Oct 16; 80(2):265-6. Abstract

Han K, Holder Jr JL, Schaaf CP, Lu H, Chen H, Kang H, Tang J, Wu Z, Hao S, Cheung SW, Yu P, Sun H, Breman AM, Patel A, Lu HC, Zoghbi HY. SHANK3 overexpression causes manic-like behaviour with unique pharmacogenetic properties. Nature. 2013 Oct 23. Abstract

 
Comments on News and Primary Papers
Primary Papers: SHANK3 overexpression causes manic-like behaviour with unique pharmacogenetic properties.

Comment by:  Francis McMahon, SRF Advisor
Submitted 31 October 2013 Posted 31 October 2013

Huda Zoghbi has been a major contributor over the years to...  Read more


View all comments by Francis McMahon

Comment by:  Kevin J. Mitchell
Submitted 7 November 2013 Posted 11 November 2013

Instead of asking whether a particular animal model is...  Read more


View all comments by Kevin J. Mitchell

Comment by:  Barbara K. Lipska
Submitted 13 November 2013 Posted 15 November 2013

There is a classic catch-22 in an attempt to model...  Read more


View all comments by Barbara K. Lipska

Comment by:  Karoly Mirnics, SRF Advisor
Submitted 12 November 2013 Posted 15 November 2013
  I recommend the Primary Papers

I think that we are often making a mistake if we directly...  Read more


View all comments by Karoly Mirnics
Submit a Comment on this News Article
Make a comment on this news article. 

If you already are a member, please login.
Not sure if you are a member? Search our member database.

*First Name  
*Last Name  
Affiliation  
Country or Territory  
*Login Email Address  
*Confirm Email Address  
*Password  
*Confirm Password  
Remember my Login and Password?  
Get SRF newsletter with recent commentary?  
 
Enter the code as it is shown below:
This code helps prevent automated registrations.

I recommend the Primary Papers

Please note: A member needs to be both registered and logged in to submit a comment.

Comment:

(If coauthors exist for this comment, please enter their names and email addresses at the end of the comment.)

References:


SRF News
SRF Comments
Text Size
Reset Text Size
Email this pageEmail this page

Share/Bookmark
Copyright © 2005- 2014 Schizophrenia Research Forum Privacy Policy Disclaimer Disclosure Copyright